Highlights

  • There was a significant unadjusted association between at-risk gambling and binge drinking in the Swedish population.
  • This association did not remain significant after controlling for confounding variables.
  • Age and daily smoking had the largest impact on this association.

Abstract

While the association between problem gambling and alcohol use disorders has been studied previously, little is known about the association between risk gambling and risk drinking. This study aimed at examining the association between at-risk gambling and binge drinking in the general Swedish population and to test whether this association remained after controlling for demographic factors. The data was part of a larger ongoing survey in the general Swedish population. Respondents (N = 19 530) were recruited through random digit dialing and interviewed about their alcohol habits (binge drinking), at-risk gambling (the Lie/Bet questionnaire) and demographics (gender, age, education, residence size, marital status, labor market status, country of origin and smoking). There was an association between lifetime at-risk gambling and current (12 months) weekly binge drinking for both men (OR = 1.73; CI 95%: 1.27–2.35) and women (OR = 2.27; CI 95%: 1.05–4.90). After controlling for demographics this association no longer remained significant (OR = 1.38; CI 95%; .99–1.90 for men and OR = 1.99; CI 95%: .94–4.66 for women). Age and smoking had the largest impact on this association. At-risk gambling and binge drinking are associated behaviors. However, it seems as if this association may be confounded by demographic variables. We hypothesize that similarities in personality profiles and health aspects could account for an additional part of the association.

Keywords

At-risk gambling;Binge drinking;Demographics

1. Introduction

Excessive gambling and drinking can yield severe consequences affecting individuals, their families and society. Gambling disorder (GD) and Alcohol Use Disorders (AUD) are commonly described as two separate syndromes (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Shaffer et al. (2004), however, suggest an addiction syndrome with common etiology underlying both substance and behavioral addictions. Both GD and AUD share common diagnostic features, such as for instance increased tolerance and withdrawal. Further, meta-analyses show that individuals with GD and individuals with Substance Use Disorders (SUD) seem to have a similar personality profile characterized by high neuroticism, disinhibition and disagreeableness, (Kotov et al., 2010 ;  Maclaren et al., 2011). The prevalence of other psychiatric disorders, such as depression, anxiety and personality disorders, has shown to be significantly higher in individuals with both GD and AUD, than in individuals with GD only (Abdollahnejad, Delfabbro, & Denson, 2014). In addition, demographic factors such as age, gender, marital status, residential size, ethnicity, education, income and employment are associated with both gambling and drinking (Johansson et al., 2009; Marsh and Dale, 2005; Matzger et al., 2004; Nalpas et al., 2011 ;  Swendsen et al., 2009). However, there seem to be gender differences and some studies have not confirmed an association between problematic gambling and drinking among females (Griffiths et al., 2010 ;  Huang et al., 2011).

The behaviors also seem to have a direct impact on each other. Among non-pathological gamblers, about 80% reported consuming four to ten drinks of alcohol during their last episode of gambling on electronic gaming machines (Baron & Dickerson, 1999). Further, alcohol consumption paired with gambling has shown to result in larger bets and greater and more rapid losses (Cronce and Corbin, 2010 ;  Giacopassi et al., 1998). In addition, hazardous drinking has been found to be one of the strongest predictors of problem gambling stability (Abbott, Williams, & Volberg, 2004).

A complication in this research field is the many terms defining excessive gambling and drinking. The two diagnoses alcohol abuse and alcohol dependence are integrated into Alcohol Use Disorders (AUD), ranging from mild to severe (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000; DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). There is no general consensus on a definition of risk drinking, but at-risk drinking (or hazardous drinking) is sometimes referred to as drinking more than 14 standard drinks per week for men, or more than seven for females, and binge drinking as drinking five drinks or more in a row for males and four or more for females (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2005). Binge drinking has been associated with injuries, car accidents, unsafe sexual activity, falls, assaults and overall poor neuropsychological functioning (Fillmore & Jude, 2011). In addition, binge drinkers have an elevated risk for developing AUD. The term pathological gambling has been replaced with Gambling Disorder (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000; DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Studies suggest that the changes in the DSM yields a higher prevalence of GD compared to pathological gambling (Rennert et al., 2014), but will only have a minimal impact on SUD prevalence (Peer et al., 2013). The broader term problem gambling is often used to also include individuals that do not fill the criteria for a diagnosis but still suffer significant consequences of their gambling (Blaszczynski and Nower, 2002 ;  Williams and Volberg, 2014). Further the term at-risk gambling is a behavior that may lead to more severe consequences — a gambler being at-risk for developing gambling problems. Often it is defined by a gambler experiencing one or two negative consequences of their gambling (Problem Gambling Research and Treatment Centre, 2011). At-risk gamblers have been found to experience higher distress level, more family problems from their gambling and higher levels of alcohol dependence than have non-problem gamblers (Marshall & Wynne, 2004),

Research on the association between problem/pathological gambling and AUD have reported large variation estimates across studies. Meta-analyses found prevalence rates ranging from 19–29% for problem gambling among treatment seeking patients with AUD and 9–73% for AUD among problem gamblers in community based samples, respectively (Cowlishaw et al., 2014 ;  Lorains et al., 2011). Another study found a stronger association between pathological gambling and AUD in groups with higher socioeconomic status (Welte, Barnes, Wieczorek, Tidwell, & Parker, 2001). However, for milder problems such as problem gambling (rather than pathological gambling) and alcohol abuse (rather than dependence), the association no longer remained statistically significant when controlling for socio-demographic variables (Kessler et al., 2008; Park et al., 2010 ;  Petry et al., 2005). Studies have found gender, education and race/ethnicity to have an impact on this association (Elia and Jacobs, 1993; Rennert et al., 2014 ;  Toneatto and Brennan, 2002).

Even though the association between AUD and problem gambling has been studied before, very few studies have examined the association between risk gambling and risk drinking. A study examining SUD in treatment seeking problem gamblers found a prevalence rate of 16.5% for risky or harmful alcohol use (Smith et al., 2010) and Bischof et al. (2013) found that 44% of at-risk gamblers also filled the criteria for AUD. Adolescent problem gamblers were significantly more likely to binge drink then non-problem gamblers, but also non-problem gamblers had a higher risk of weekly binge drinking than individuals who did not gamble at all (Walker, Clark, & Folk, 2010). In Sweden, nearly 55% of problem gamblers had risky drinking habits, whereas 13% with risky alcohol habits were also at- risk gamblers (Swedish National Institute of Public Health, 2010).

Even though risk behaviors affect a substantially larger group than diagnostic conditions (e.g. AUD and GD), studies on the association between at-risk gambling and risk drinking are sparse. To our knowledge and our surprise, we found no published study examining the association between at-risk gambling and risk drinking. Furthermore, the influence of demographics is overlooked at times. Therefore, this study aimed at examining the association between at-risk gambling and binge drinking in the general Swedish population, controlling for relevant demographic variables.

The aim of this study was to examine the association between lifetime at-risk gambling and current (12 months) binge drinking in the general Swedish population and to test whether this association remained after controlling for confounding variables.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

This cross-sectional study is part of the larger, ongoing so called Monitoring project (Ramstedt, Lindell, & Raninen, 2013). The Monitoring project aims at estimating alcohol- and tobacco use in the Swedish population and the data is used as a basis for the official alcohol statistics in Sweden. The sampling, through random-digit dialing, and interviewing, carried out using Computer Aided Telephone Interviews (CATI), are conducted by a commercial company (Ipsos) specialized in performing telephone interviews (Raninen, Leifman, & Ramstedt, 2013). The Monitoring project has been evaluated by an independent expert group who concluded that the methods of the project were satisfying (Ramstedt, Sohlberg, Engdahl, & Svensson, 2009). The Monitoring project has been previously described (Leifman and Trolldal, 2013; Ramstedt et al., 2013; Ramstedt, 2010 ;  Raninen et al., 2013) and will only be summarized here. Every month 1500 randomly assigned respondents answer questions about their alcohol and tobacco habits. Multiple (30) contact attempts are made before it is coded as a non-response (Ramstedt, 2010). From April 2012 until May 2013, all participants were also screened for at-risk gambling. Accordingly, this represents the time frame for the database to the present study.

The participants consisted of 19,530 randomly selected, nationally representative Swedish residents. Out of the 19,530, 54% were female and 46% male. They were between 16–82 years old with a mean age of 50. In total, 40% had a university education, 67% were married or cohabiting and 92% were born in Sweden. In total, 3.1% reported lifetime at-risk gambling and 4.4% current weekly binge drinking.

The monthly non-response is about 60% during the study period. A respondent not being reached or declining participation is replaced, so that 1500 individuals are interviewed every month. A previous study of 2500 non-responders that were re-contacted a year later, found no significant differences in alcohol habits between those and responders answering at the first occasion (Wennberg, Svensson, & Ramstedt, 2011). Though, the proportion of abstainers was significantly higher among the initial non-responders.

2.2. Measures

Respondents were screened for at-risk gambling using the Lie/Bet questionnaire (Johnson et al., 1997). Respondents reporting that they had, 1. lied to people important to them about how much they gambled and/or 2. felt the need to bet more and more money, were classified as lifetime at-risk gamblers. A previous study conclude that the Lie/Bet screening showed both high sensitivity (.92) and specificity (.96) for screening problem and pathological gamblers in a community sample (Götestam, Johansson, Wenzel, & Simonsen, 2004). The respondents screening positive on one of the Lie/Bet questions, and accepting to participate in an upcoming study, were sent a postal survey (Sundqvist & Wennberg, 2014) including the short version of the National Opinion Research Center DSM-IV Screen for Gambling Problems (NODS). The short version NODS-PERC, consists of four of the originally 17 questions (Volberg, Abbott, Rönnberg, & Munck, 2001). The authors found the combination of the four questions about Preoccupation, Escape, Risked relationships and Chasing (PERC) to best predict problem gambling. A majority of the respondents were not classified as problem or pathological gamblers according to the PERC and hence, at-risk gambling seems as an accurate definition of this group.

To screen for binge drinking the respondents were asked: During the last 12 months, how often did you at the same occasion drink alcohol equivalent to at least a bottle of wine (75 cl), or 5 glasses of strong spirits (25 cl), or 4 cans of strong beer or strong cider (> 3.5 percentage per volume), or 6 cans of medium strong beer (3.5 percentage per volume). Also consider all the occasions when you combined different types of alcohol and try to report how often you drunk an equivalent amount. The response categories were a gradient ranging from ‘more or less every day’, ‘4–5 times a week’, ‘2–3 times a week’, ‘once a week’, ‘about 2–3 times’, a few times, ‘about once’ to ‘never’.

Individuals reporting consuming alcohol equivalent to at least one bottle of wine, 25 cl of spirits or for cans of beer per occasion every week or more often during the past 12 months were classified as current weekly binge drinkers.

Further, respondents were also asked about their marital status, smoking habits, monthly income, educational level, contemporary occupation, city of residence and if they were born in or outside Sweden. Residential size was divided into large (The three largest cities in Sweden; Stockholm, Gothenburg and Malmö), medium (> 100,000 residents within 100 km) or small (< 100,000 residents within 100 km). Occupational status was categorized into employed/retired/other, student or unemployed. Marital status was categorized into living together with someone as a partner or not. Smoking habits was categorized into daily smoking or not, educational level into completed university or not and an income less than 10,000 SEK/month (about 1200 USD or 1080 EUR).

2.3. Data analyses

Demographics variables known in previous research (Johansson et al., 2009; Marsh and Dale, 2005; Matzger et al., 2004; Nalpas et al., 2011 ;  Swendsen et al., 2009) to predict or to be likely to predict GD and AUD, and admitted for in the Monitoring project, were chosen as confounding variables in the analyses (gender, age, education, residence size, marital status, occupational status, country of origin and smoking). Even though smoking is not a demographic variable it is a behavior that is highly comorbid with both problem gambling and excessive drinking (Harrison et al., 2008 ;  McGrath and Barrett, 2009) and was therefore chosen to be included as a potential confounder. All variables are defined as demographics in order to simplify. The variable “income” was excluded due to high number of missing cases.

To check for multicollinearity, Variance Inflation Factors (VIF) were calculated through linear Regression where all variables were included. All VIF-scores were between 1.01 and 1.06 and multicollinearity is therefore not likely to be a problem. To examine the association between lifetime at-risk gambling and current weekly binge drinking (12 months), multiple logistic regression models were computed. Model 1 was unadjusted since we were interested in the overall association. Model 2 was adjusted for age since younger are known to both gamble and drink to a greater extent. Model 3 was adjusted for age and smoking. Since smoking is not a demographic variable per se, we think it is interesting to see the impact of this factor. Finally a Full Model adjusted for all demographic variables was calculated. The analyses were stratified on gender, but not on age since that yielded too few cases in some cells. Data were analyzed using SPSS version 22.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic characteristics

Differences between lifetime at-risk gamblers and non-risk gamblers are described in Table 1. Male at-risk gamblers were to a higher extent (then male non-risk gamblers) characterized by young age, daily smoking, living alone, lower education, being born outside of Sweden, living in a big city and not carrying an employment. For women, the pattern was similar, but there were smaller differences between the groups regarding age and residential size. Among women, nearly twice as many at-risk gamblers as non-risk gamblers were unemployed.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics by lifetime at-risk gambling among a representative adult population. N = 19,530.
Demographic characteristics Men (n = 9015) Women (n = 10,515)
At-risk gambling n = 427 Not at-risk gambling n = 8588 At-risk gambling n = 180 Not at-risk gambling n = 10,335
Age (M, SD) 41.8 (18.1) 50.1 (17.7) 48.6 (17.9) 51.9 (17.5)
 Smoking habits (%)
  Daily smoker 16.4 7.1 14.4 9.5
  Non daily smoker 83.6 92.9 85.6 90.5
  Missing (n) 0 8 0 3
 Marital status (%)
  Living without partner 38.9 30.2 37.8 34.7
  Living with partner 61.1 69.8 62.2 65.3
  Missing (n) 0 5 0 6
 Educational level (%)
  No university 71.3 64.6 59.2 55.7
  University 28.7 35.4 40.8 44.3
  Missing (n) 2 42 1 53
 Place of origin (%)
  Born outside Sweden 10.3 7.1 12.8 8.7
  Born in Sweden 89.7 92.9 87.2 91.3
  Missing (n) 1 23 1 15
 Residential size (%)
  Big city 40.0 31.5 33.9 33.4
  Middle size city 50.6 57.5 53.9 55.5
  Small size city 9.4 11.0 12.2 11.0
  Missing (n) 0 1 0 2
 Labor market status (%)
  Unemployed 3.8 2.7 4.5 2.3
  Student 12.0 7.8 11.2 7.6
  Employed, retired, other 84.2 89.5 84.3 90.1
  Missing (n) 2 15 1 33

3.2. The association between at-risk gambling and binge drinking

Table 2 presents the association between lifetime at-risk gambling and current weekly binge drinking (12 months) separated on gender. Model 1 shows the unadjusted estimates while model 2 shows estimates adjusted only for age. In model 3 the estimates are adjusted for both age and smoking. Finally, in model 4 (Full Model), the estimates are adjusted for all demographic variables as well as for smoking.

Table 2. The association between lifetime at-risk gambling and past year binge drinking measured as odds ratios (OR) with corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CI 95%). N = 19,530.
Model 1 OR CI 95% Model 2 OR CI 95% Model 3 OR CI 95% Model 4 OR CI 95%
Men n = 9015
 Risk gambling 1.73** 1.27–2.35 1.53** 1.11–2.09 1.38* 1.01–1.91 1.38 0.99–1.90
Women n = 10,515
 Risk gambling 2.27** 1.05–4.90 2.15 .98–4.72 1.99 .90–4.40 2.10 .94–4.66

Model 1: Unadjusted.

Model 2: Adjusted for age.

Model 3: Adjusted for age and smoking.

Model 4: Adjusted for age, smoking, marital status, residential size, educational level, labor market status and place of origin.

p < .05.

⁎⁎p < .01.

Lifetime at-risk gamblers had more frequently been binge drinking during the last 12 months (11.8% for men and 3.9% for women) than non-risk gamblers (7.2% for men and 1.8% for women), (OR = 1.73; CI 95%: 1.27–2.35 for men and OR = 2.27; CI 95%: 1.05–4.90 for women). After adjusting for age (model 2), this association remained statistically significant, though weakened, for men (OR = 1.53; CI 95%: 1.11–2.09). For women the association was no longer statistically significant (OR = 2.15; CI 95%: .98–4.72). The association weakened further after adjusting for age and smoking (model 3). When adjusting for all variables included in model 4 (Full model), the association between lifetime at-risk gambling and current weekly binge drinking no longer remained statistically significant, neither for men (OR = 1.38; CI 95%: .99–1.90), nor for women (OR = 2.10; CI 95%: .94–4.66). Age and being a daily smoker had the largest impact on the association for both men and women. For men there is basically no difference in the estimate and only marginally in the confidence interval after the adjustment in the full model. For women on the other hand, there seems to be a small negative confounding as a result for the adjustment in the full model.

In conclusion, lifetime at-risk gamblers had a substantially higher risk for weekly binge drinking during the last year. However, this only held true when not controlling for confounding variables, mainly age and smoking.

4. Discussion

Our results indicate that individuals with lifetime at-risk gambling have substantially higher likelihood of current weekly binge drinking than individuals with no at-risk gambling. In the group of at-risk gamblers 11.8% of men and 3.9% of women were binge drinking every week. However, this association no longer remained statistically significant after controlling for relevant confounding variables. Age and smoking had the greatest impact on the association between at-risk gambling and binge drinking.

In prior research, an association has been found between GD and AUD, as well as between problem gambling and alcohol abuse (Cowlishaw et al., 2014 ;  Lorains et al., 2011). To a great extent, the latter seems to be due to socio-demographic similarities (Kessler et al., 2008; Park et al., 2010 ;  Petry et al., 2005). This is in line with what was found in this study on risk behaviors in the general population. It is possible that other common risk factors for excessive gambling and drinking, such as shared personality profiles and health aspects (Johansson et al., 2009; Marsh and Dale, 2005 ;  Sundqvist and Wennberg, 2014), could be an additional explanation for the association between the behaviors. If this is true it would mean that the association could be mainly due to shared characteristics rather than a causal link between the behaviors. That would be in line with the theory of an addiction syndrome (Shaffer et al., 2004). This model suggests addiction being a unitary disorder with a variety of expressions. Our results indicate that the same line of thinking could be applied on a sub-clinical level, including at-risk gambling and binge drinking.

As described in the introduction, the heterogeneity in prevalence ratings for co-occurring GD and AUD is large across studies. Prevalence rates range from 9–73% for AUD among problem gamblers (Lorains et al., 2011) and 19–29% for problem gambling among AUD (Cowlishaw et al., 2014). Part of this could be explained by different sample characteristics. For example, a sample characterized by younger, smoking males with a lower socio-demographic status and living in a big city would yield a higher co-occurrence then a sample with non-smoking, highly educated women.

A limitation in the study is the large proportion of non-responders. A study within this project found no significant differences in alcohol habits between 2500 non-responders re-contacted a year later compared to responders answering at the first occasion, (Wennberg et al., 2011). Nevertheless, a group of “hard” non-responders (not responding despite extensive effort) remain unstudied. We believe that this group includes a higher proportion with both excessive gambling and drinking, and consequently that we underestimate the prevalence. Studies on non-responders have found an underestimation on risk behaviors, but also non-responders to be younger, male and with lower socio-economic status (Maclennan et al., 2012 ;  Meiklejohn et al., 2012). If this holds true in this study, this could mean that the association between at-risk gambling and binge drinking might not be significantly different if non-responders were included. Another limitation is the use of only two questions for screening at-risk gamblers. It is likely that the use of more items would have yielded a higher proportion of at-risk gamblers or even some problematic gamblers. Further, the study design did not admit to also include personality profiles and psychiatric comorbidity, variables that probably would have an additional impact on the association between at-risk gambling and binge drinking. However, a major strength of the study is the recruitment of a large nationally representative sample from the general population, as well as and the focus on risk behaviors. This is warranted as a complement to studies focusing on problematic/disordered behaviors in a clinical or student setting. An alternative approach of studying the association between excessive gambling and drinking could be to explore different prevalence rates for different demographic profiles. A study including both personality profiles, health factors (e.g. comorbidity) and demographic factors could further help understand the association between gambling and drinking behaviors on different problem levels.

In conclusion, the association between at-risk gambling and binge drinking did not maintain significant when controlling for common demographics influencing the association. We hypothesize that personality profiles and health aspects are other important confounders, explaining an additional part of the association between excessive gambling and drinking. If this holds true, a psychosocial profile associated with at-risk gambling, rather than at-risk gambling per see, is associated with an increased risk of binge drinking.

Author disclosure

Funding for this study was provided by Svenska Spel. Svenska Spel had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

K.S. and P.W. designed the study and wrote the protocol. K.S. conducted literature researches and provided summaries of previous research studies. K.S. conducted the statistical analysis. I.R. contributed with statistical and epidemiological advising. K.S. wrote the first draft of the manuscript and all three authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

All three authors declare no conflicts of interest.

We are grateful for insightful comments on the manuscript by professor Tomas Hemmingsson, professor Jan Blomqvist and PhD Anders Ledberg.

Acknowledgment

The study was funded by Svenska Spel. We declare no conflict of interest. We are grateful for insightful comments on the manuscript by professor Tomas Hemmingsson, professor Jan Blomqvist and PhD Anders Ledberg.

References

  1. Abbott et al., 2004 M.W. Abbott, M.M. Williams, R.A. Volberg; A prospective study of problem and regular nonproblem gamblers living in the community; Substance Use & Misuse, 39 (6) (2004), pp. 855–884 https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120030891
  2. Abdollahnejad et al., 2014 R. Abdollahnejad, P. Delfabbro, L. Denson; Psychiatric co-morbidity in problem and pathological gamblers: investigating the confounding influence of alcohol use disorder; Addictive Behaviors, 39 (3) (2014), pp. 566–572 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.11.004
  3. American Psychiatric Association, 2000 American Psychiatric Association; Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision. Text (Vol. Washington); American Psychiatric Publishing, Arlington, VA (2000)
  4. American Psychiatric Association, 2013 American Psychiatric Association; Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders; American Journal of Psychiatry ((5th Ed.))American Psychiatric Publishing, Arlington, VA (2013)
  5. Baron and Dickerson, 1999 E. Baron, M. Dickerson; Alcohol consumption and self-control of gambling behaviour; Journal of Gambling Studies/Co-Sponsored by the National Council on Problem Gambling and Institute for the Study of Gambling and Commercial Gaming, 15 (1) (1999), pp. 3–15 (Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12766451)
  6. Bischof et al., 2013 A. Bischof, C. Meyer, G. Bischof, N. Kastirke, U. John, H.J. Rumpf; Comorbid Axis I-disorders among subjects with pathological, problem, or at-risk gambling recruited from the general population in Germany: results of the PAGE study; Psychiatry Research, 210 (3) (2013), pp. 1065–1070 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.07.026
  7. Blaszczynski and Nower, 2002 A. Blaszczynski, L. Nower; A pathway model of problem and pathological gambling; Addiction (Abingdon, England), 97 (5) (2002), pp. 487–499 https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00015.x
  8. Cowlishaw et al., 2014 S. Cowlishaw, S. Merkouris, A. Chapman, H. Radermacher; Pathological and problem gambling in substance use treatment: a systematic review and meta-analysis; Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 46 (2) (2014), pp. 98–105 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsat.2013.08.019
  9. Cronce and Corbin, 2010 J.M. Cronce, W.R. Corbin; Effects of alcohol and initial gambling outcomes on within-session gambling behavior; Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 18 (2) (2010), pp. 145–157 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019114
  10. Elia and Jacobs, 1993 C. Elia, D.F. Jacobs; The incidence of pathological gambling among Native Americans treated for alcohol dependence; The International Journal of the Addictions, 28 (7) (1993), pp. 659–666
  11. Fillmore and Jude, 2011 M.T. Fillmore, R. Jude; Defining “binge” drinking as five drinks per occasion or drinking to a.08% BAC: which is more sensitive to risk?; American Journal on Addictions, 20 (2011), pp. 468–475 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1521-0391.2011.00156.x
  12. Giacopassi et al., 1998 D. Giacopassi, B.G. Stitt, M. Vandiver; An analysis of the relationship of alcohol to casino gambling among college students; Journal of Gambling Studies/Co-Sponsored by the National Council on Problem Gambling and Institute for the Study of Gambling and Commercial Gaming, 14 (1998), pp. 135–149 (Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12766439)
  13. Götestam et al., 2004 K.G. Götestam, A. Johansson, H.G. Wenzel, I.-E. Simonsen; Validation of the lie/bet screen for pathological gambling on two normal population data sets; Psychological Reports, 95 (3 Pt 1) (2004), pp. 1009–1013 https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.95.3.1009-1013
  14. Griffiths et al., 2010 M. Griffiths, H. Wardle, J. Orford, K. Sproston, B. Erens; Gambling, alcohol, consumption, cigarette smoking and health: findings from the 2007 British Gambling Prevalence Survey; Addiction Research & Theory, 18 (2) (2010), pp. 208–223 https://doi.org/10.3109/16066350902928569
  15. Harrison et al., 2008 E.L.R. Harrison, R.A. Desai, S.A. McKee; Nondaily smoking and alcohol use, hazardous drinking, and alcohol diagnoses among young adults: findings from the NESARC; Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32 (12) (2008), pp. 2081–2087 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00796.x
  16. Huang et al., 2011 J.-H. Huang, D.F. Jacobs, J.L. Derevensky; DSM-based problem gambling: increasing the odds of heavy drinking in a national sample of U.S. college athletes?; Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45 (3) (2011), pp. 302–308 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.07.001
  17. Johansson et al., 2009 A. Johansson, J.E. Grant, S.W. Kim, B.L. Odlaug, K.G. Götestam; Risk factors for problematic gambling: a critical literature review; Journal of Gambling Studies/Co-Sponsored by the National Council on Problem Gambling and Institute for the Study of Gambling and Commercial Gaming, 25 (2009), pp. 67–92 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-008-9088-6
  18. Johnson et al., 1997 E.E. Johnson, R. Hamer, R.M. Nora, B. Tan, N. Eisenstein, C. Engelhart; The Lie/Bet Questionnaire for screening pathological gamblers; Psychological Reports, 80 (1997), pp. 83–88 https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1998.83.3f.1219
  19. Kessler et al., 2008 R.C. Kessler, I. Hwang, R. LaBrie, M. Petukhova, N.A. Sampson, K.C. Winters, H.J. Shaffer; DSM-IV pathological gambling in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication; Psychological Medicine, 38 (9) (2008), pp. 1351–1360 https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291708002900
  20. Kotov et al., 2010 R. Kotov, W. Gamez, F. Schmidt, D. Watson; Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis; Psychological Bulletin, 136 (5) (2010), pp. 768–821 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020327
  21. Leifman and Trolldal, 2013 H. Leifman, B. Trolldal; Alkoholkonsumtionen i Sverige 2013; edish Council for Information on Alcohol and Other Drugs (CAN), Stockholm (2013)
  22. Lorains et al., 2011 F.K. Lorains, S. Cowlishaw, S.A. Thomas; Prevalence of comorbid disorders in problem and pathological gambling: systematic review and meta-analysis of population surveys; Addiction (Abingdon, England), 106 (3) (2011), pp. 490–498 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03300.x
  23. Maclaren et al., 2011 V.V. Maclaren, J.A. Fugelsang, K.A. Harrigan, M.J. Dixon; The personality of pathological gamblers: a meta-analysis; Clinical Psychology Review, 31 (6) (2011), pp. 1057–1067 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.02.002
  24. Maclennan et al., 2012 B. Maclennan, K. Kypri, J. Langley, R. Room; Non-response bias in a community survey of drinking, alcohol-related experiences and public opinion on alcohol policy; Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 126 (1-2) (2012), pp. 189–194 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.05.014
  25. Marsh and Dale, 2005 A. Marsh, A. Dale; Risk factors for alcohol and other drug disorders: a review; Australian Psychologist, 40 (2) (2005), pp. 73–80 https://doi.org/10.1080/00050060500094662
  26. Marshall and Wynne, 2004 K. Marshall, H. Wynne; Against the odds: a profile of at-risk and problem gamblers; Canadian Social Trends, 4 (73) (2004), pp. 25–29 (Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com.proxy1.lib.umanitoba.ca/docview/224106075/139B6C73530135A32A4/3?accountid=14569)
  27. Matzger et al., 2004 H. Matzger, K. Delucchi, C. Weisner, L. Ammon; Does marital status predict long-term drinking? Five-year observations of dependent and problem drinkers; Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 65 (2) (2004), pp. 255–265
  28. McGrath and Barrett, 2009 D.S. McGrath, S.P. Barrett; The comorbidity of tobacco smoking and gambling: a review of the literature; Drug and Alcohol Review, 28 (6) (2009), pp. 676–681 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1465-3362.2009.00097.x
  29. Meiklejohn et al., 2012 J. Meiklejohn, J. Connor, K. Kypri; The effect of low survey response rates on estimates of alcohol consumption in a general population survey; PLoS ONE, 7 (4) (2012), pp. 1–6 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0035527
  30. Nalpas et al., 2011 B. Nalpas, J. Yguel, B. Fleury, S. Martin, D. Jarraud, M. Craplet; Pathological gambling in treatment-seeking alcoholics: a national survey in France; Alcohol and Alcoholism, 46 (2) (2011), pp. 156–160 https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agq099
  31. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2005 National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; Screening for alcohol use and alcohol-related problems, 65 (2005)
  32. Park et al., 2010 S. Park, M.J. Cho, H.J. Jeon, H.W. Lee, J.N. Bae, J.I. Park, J.P. Hong; Prevalence, clinical correlations, comorbidities, and suicidal tendencies in pathological Korean gamblers: results from the Korean Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study; Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 45 (6) (2010), pp. 621–629 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-009-0102-9
  33. Peer et al., 2013 K. Peer, L. Rennert, K.G. Lynch, L. Farrer, J. Gelernter, H.R. Kranzler; Prevalence of DSM-IV and DSM-5 alcohol, cocaine, opioid, and cannabis use disorders in a largely substance dependent sample; Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 127 (1-3) (2013), pp. 215–219 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.07.009
  34. Petry et al., 2005 N.M. Petry, F.S. Stinson, B.F. Grant; Comorbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other psychiatric disorders: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions; The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66 (5) (2005), pp. 564–574 (Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15889941)
  35. Problem Gambling Research and Treatment Centre, (PGRTC), 2011 Problem Gambling Research and Treatment Centre, (PGRTC); Guideline for Screening, Assessment and Treatment in Problem Gambling; Monash University, Melbourne (2011)
  36. Ramstedt, 2010 M. Ramstedt; How much alcohol do you buy? A comparison of self-reported alcohol purchases with actual sales; Addiction, 105 (2010), pp. 649–654 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02839.x
  37. Ramstedt et al., 2013 M. Ramstedt, A. Lindell, J. Raninen; Tal om alkohol 2012 — en statistisk årsrapport från Monitorprojektet. (SoRADs Rapportserie, nr 67). Stockholm; (2013)
  38. Ramstedt et al., 2009 M. Ramstedt, T.A. Sohlberg, B. Engdahl, J. Svensson; Tal om tobak 2008 Tobakskonsumtionen i Sverige 2008; (2009)
  39. Raninen et al., 2013 J. Raninen, H. Leifman, M. Ramstedt; Who is not drinking less in Sweden? An analysis of the decline in consumption for the period 2004–2011; Alcohol and Alcoholism, 48 (5) (2013), pp. 592–597 https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agt051
  40. Rennert et al., 2014 L. Rennert, C. Denis, K. Peer, K.G. Lynch, J. Gelernter, H.R. Kranzler; DSM-5 gambling disorder: prevalence and characteristics in a substance use disorder sample; Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 22 (1) (2014), pp. 50–56 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034518
  41. Shaffer et al., 2004 H.J. Shaffer, D.A. LaPlante, R.A. LaBrie, R.C. Kidman, A.N. Donato, M.V. Stanton; Toward a syndrome model of addiction: multiple expressions, common etiology; Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 12 (6) (2004), pp. 367–374 https://doi.org/10.1080/10673220490905705
  42. Smith et al., 2010 D. Smith, P. Harvey, M. Battersby, R. Pols, J. Oakes, M. Baigent; Treatment outcomes and predictors of drop out for problem gamblers in South Australia: a cohort study; The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 44 (10) (2010), pp. 911–920 https://doi.org/10.3109/00048674.2010.493502
  43. Sundqvist and Wennberg, 2014 K. Sundqvist, P. Wennberg; Risk gambling and personality: results from a representative Swedish sample; Journal of Gambling Studies (2014) https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-014-9473-2
  44. Swedish National Institute of Public Health, 2010 Swedish National Institute of Public Health; Spel om pengar och spelproblem i Sverige 2008/2009; Statens Folkhälsoinstitut, Östersund (2010)
  45. Swendsen et al., 2009 J. Swendsen, K.P. Conway, L. Degenhardt, L. Dierker, M. Glantz, R. Jin, R.C. Kessler; Socio-demographic risk factors for alcohol and drug dependence: the 10-year follow-up of the national comorbidity survey; Addiction, 104 (8) (2009), pp. 1346–1355 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02622.x
  46. Toneatto and Brennan, 2002 T. Toneatto, J. Brennan; Pathological gambling in treatment-seeking substance abusers; Addictive Behaviors, 27 (3) (2002), pp. 465–469 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4603(00)00173-8
  47. Volbeg et al., 2001 R.A. Volbeg, M.W. Abbott, S. Rönnberg, I.M. Munch; Prevalence and risk of pathological gambling in Sweden; Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 104 (2001), pp. 250–256
  48. Walker et al., 2010 D. Walker, C. Clark, J. Folk; The relationship between gambling behavior and binge drinking, hard drug use, and paying for sex; Gaming Research & Review Journal, 14 (1) (2010), pp. 15–26 (Retrieved from http://digitalscholarship.unlv.edu/grrj/vol14/iss1/2/)
  49. Welte et al., 2001 J. Welte, G. Barnes, W. Wieczorek, M.C. Tidwell, J. Parker; Alcohol and gambling pathology among U.S. adults: prevalence, demographic patterns and comorbidity; Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 62 (5) (2001), pp. 706–712 (Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11702810)
  50. Wennberg et al., 2011 P. Wennberg, J. Svensson, M. Ramstedt; The effects of missing data when surveying alcohol habits; Nordic Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 28 (1) (2011), pp. 43–50 https://doi.org/10.2478/v10199-011-0004-5
  51. Williams and Volberg, 2014 R.J. Williams, R.A. Volberg; The classification accuracy of four problem gambling assessment instruments in population research; International Gambling Studies, 14 (1) (2014), pp. 15–28 https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2013.839731
Back to Top

Document information

Published on 26/05/17
Submitted on 26/05/17

Licence: Other

Document Score

0

Views 6
Recommendations 0

Share this document

claim authorship

Are you one of the authors of this document?